Science says rats can be kind. Here鈥檚 why that matters.
Far from merciless, rats, it seems, will go out of their way to avoid harming each other. The animal kingdom may be more empathetic than people think.
Far from merciless, rats, it seems, will go out of their way to avoid harming each other. The animal kingdom may be more empathetic than people think.
At the end of the Second World War, U.S. Army Brigadier General S.L.A. Marshall spoke with thousands of infantry soldiers and uncovered a surprising facet of human nature: fewer than 1 in 5 soldiers had actually fired their guns in combat, even though 4 in 5 had the chance to do so. Many troops, he found, had aimed high or off to the side to avoid hitting the enemy.聽
鈥淭he average and normal healthy individual,鈥 wrote General Marshall in 1947, 鈥渉as such an inner and unrealized resistance toward killing another man that he will not of his own volition take life if it is possible to turn away from that responsibility.鈥
Today, psychologists use the term 鈥渉arm aversion鈥 to describe our seemingly innate and widespread unwillingness to make another human, even a total stranger, suffer. But where does this reluctance come from? Is it something we learn as children? Or does it run deeper?聽
A study published March 5 in the journal Current Biology suggests that it has roots in the very thing we often use to justify our selfishness: evolution.
Researchers at the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences and the University of Amsterdam found that some rats display an unwillingness to harm their fellow rats, even when it benefits them to do so. This unwillingness, the researchers found, correlates to a similar聽biological mechanism in humans, suggesting that this innate pacifism emerged among mammals more than a million centuries ago.
鈥淣ext time you call someone a rat, to suggest a low moral quality, think again!鈥 writes neuroscientist 海角大神 Keysers, a senior author of the study, in an email to the Monitor.聽
Getting out of the rat race
The research of Dr. Keysers and his colleagues helps explain the evolutionary origins of 鈥減rosociality,鈥 behavior that benefits conspecifics, that is, other members of one鈥檚 own species, without an obvious evolutionary benefit to oneself.聽
鈥淓volutionary biology might, at first sight, suggest that animals are selfish in the struggle for survival,鈥 writes Dr. Keysers. 鈥淎fter all, conspecifics are the worst competitors, as they use the exact same resources we use.鈥
To test the rats鈥 moral fiber, Dr. Keysers and his colleagues put 24 rats of both sexes through a series of experiments. First, they trained the rats to press two levers, each of which delivered a sucrose pellet, until they had developed a preference for one lever. Then, the experimenters wired the preferred lever so that, when a rat pressed it, a rat in a neighboring cage would receive an electric shock.聽聽
Nine of the 24 rats immediately stopped pressing the preferred lever upon witnessing their neighbor get shocked, regardless of whether it was a rat they shared their home cage with or a complete stranger.聽
鈥淚n a way, the individual variability was really stronger than we expected,鈥 writes Dr. Keysers.聽Perhaps rats are as distinct from each other as humans are, he adds.
Creatures of habit聽
Next, the experimenters rigged the shocking lever to deliver two pellets. The rats that had stopped pushing their preferred lever still tended to steer clear of it, pushing the one-pellet lever instead, sacrificing one pellet to avoid harming their neighbors.聽But, as with humans, every rat has a price: When the shocking lever delivered three pellets, the rats would switch back.
鈥淲hat the rats are telling us is, 鈥業鈥檓 willing to spend one food pellet on this individual鈥檚 distress, but not two pellets,鈥欌 says University of Chicago neurobiologist Peggy Mason, who was not involved in the study.
The experimenters also 鈥渙vertrained鈥 some of the rats to keep pushing their preferred lever. They found that overtrained rats would seldom switch from their levers to avoid shocking their neighbor.聽
This is similar to the conclusion that the U.S. military reached after General Marshall鈥檚 report that just 20% of troops had fired on the enemy: By deliberately desensitizing trainees to killing, for instance by using humanoid targets, the Pentagon increased the shoot-to-kill rate among U.S. troops to 55% in the Korean War, and 95% in Vietnam.聽
鈥淎 cute trick鈥
The research into rats鈥 brains serves as evidence that humans and rats share the same basic emotions, he says. This 鈥渕akes it unlikely that harm aversion evolved independently in the two species,鈥 writes Dr. Keysers. 鈥淚nstead, it suggests that it evolved before humans and rats separated 100 million years ago.鈥
This means not only that we should expect harm aversion to be widespread among mammals, but also that the emotions associated with being unwilling to cause suffering 鈥 empathy, remorse 鈥 are actually the same thing in humans and nonhumans.
鈥淣ot so long ago, it would have been taboo to even say we are researching emotions in animals,鈥 writes Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal, a neurobiologist at Tel-Aviv University, in an email to the Monitor. But recently, 鈥減eople have started understanding that taking emotions 鈥榦ff the table鈥 scientifically speaking is a case of throwing the baby out with the bathwater.鈥
Biologists say that emotions evolved because they motivate us to perform behaviors that are beneficial to our reproductive success. Prosocial emotions like empathy motivate us to benefit others in our group.
鈥淭he evolutionary advantages of having a cohesive group are so high that mammals have evolved a way to yoke one individual鈥檚 distress to another,鈥 says Dr. Mason. 鈥淚t鈥檚 a cute trick.鈥
In the case of the rats, we don鈥檛 know whether the ones who switched levers did so out of a sense of higher moral purpose, or whether their switching was simply an effort to relieve their own personal discomfort at witnessing another鈥檚 suffering. (Think of the way a screaming baby might make you squirm in your airline seat.)
鈥淚t might be that rats avoid harm to others because they care about the wellbeing of others,鈥 writes Dr. Keysers, 鈥渙r because they dislike the feeling triggered by hearing the other animal squeak.鈥
But, he adds, 鈥淲hat many people get wrong, is that they entertain the more conservative interpretation when it comes to animals, but the more altruistic interpretation when humans help other humans.鈥