Have you heard the buzz? Honeybees can count.
Loading...
What鈥檚 the first image that comes to mind when you think of mathematics? Perhaps it鈥檚 formulas scribbled on a chalkboard. Or maybe architectural drawings. Or rocket trajectories. Or taxes. Regardless, there is likely a human hand behind it.
But researchers say that humans might not be the only number crunchers in the animal kingdom. Honeybees may also be able to add and subtract, according to聽聽published Wednesday in the journal Science Advances.
And it鈥檚 not the first study to find evidence that nonhuman animals have minds for numbers.听 search for prey items stolen from their webs, suggesting that they keep running tallies.听Some聽聽seem to keep a head count of their friends. And聽聽(and monkeys) can rank quantities from smallest to largest.
Why We Wrote This
Math is often considered a uniquely human construct. But new evidence that bees can calculate simple addition and subtraction is pushing thinking about abstract thought.
Scientists say pinning down the distinction between humans鈥 and other animals鈥 numerical abilities may help us better understand ourselves, and how our species came to rule the world.
鈥淥ften when we look at animals and their intelligence, it gives us big insights into how we understand ourselves and the world we live in,鈥澛爏ays Adrian Dyer, a study author and an associate professor at RMIT University in Melbourne, Australia.听
There is some controversy over animal arithmetic discoveries. Do they actually understand numbers like we do? Or is it more of an ability to understand relative quantities? Sorting out these questions could help scientists figure out just how special human mathematics can be.
The new bee study falls right in that controversial sweet spot. Study authors termed what the bees did addition and subtraction. But some scientists say that calling it mathematics may be overinterpreting the results.
In the experiment, the Apis mellifera bees were placed in a Y-shaped maze. Upon entering the stem of the Y, the bees encountered a set of either blue or yellow shapes. Blue indicated the problem would require the bees to add one shape, yellow to subtract one. When they approached the fork in the Y, the bees then had to choose between two more sets of shapes. The correct choice was either one less or one more than the original display, depending on the color. The incorrect choice could be any other quantity from 1 to 5.
Nearly 7 times out of 10, the bees chose correctly. The results suggest the bees were doing more than simply identifying which was 鈥渕ore鈥 or 鈥渓ess,鈥 says Professor Dyer, as both options could be greater than the original display for addition problems (vice versa for subtraction) and they鈥檇 still pick the correct answer.
What even is math?
But聽Rafael N煤帽ez, director of the Embodied Cognition Laboratory at the University of California San Diego, says that this doesn鈥檛 necessarily indicate that the bees can comprehend聽numbers. Rather, he says, it shows that they are capable of discriminating between聽quantities.
It鈥檚 an issue of exactness, he says. He suspects the bees are using what he calls 鈥渜uantical cognition鈥 rather than numerical. The difference, he says, is similar to the difference between the words like 鈥渃ouple,鈥 鈥渇ew,鈥 鈥渟everal,鈥 and 鈥渙ne,鈥 鈥渢wo,鈥 鈥渢hree,鈥 鈥渇our,鈥 鈥渇ive.鈥
Another way to put it is that the bees are using approximate numbers, says聽Elizabeth Brannon, who studies comparative cognition in monkeys and both adult and child humans at the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia. And, she says, that鈥檚 part of what scientists think is one of the biggest distinctions between nonhuman animals鈥 relationship to numbers and humans鈥.
鈥淭he main hallmark of numerical discrimination in animals,鈥 Professor Brannon says, has to do with ratios. 鈥淲hen animals are comparing two numerical values, they鈥檙e much better if they differ by a large ratio than if they differ by a very small ratio.鈥
Brannon herself trained monkeys to do addition and subtraction, with a similar method to the bee study. And she found that the monkeys could get the correct answer only if the ratio between the two choices was significant enough. For example, she explains, the monkeys could get the correct answer to 17 minus 7 if the choices were 10 and 5, but not if they were 10 and 9.
It remains unclear whether that鈥檚 an issue for honeybees, but the experiment did bring into question another idea about what separates human mathematics from animals鈥 arithmetic.
The use of symbols to聽denote specific numbers and mathematical functions was once thought to be unique to humans.
But this new study suggests that honeybees might be capable of using symbols, too, as the insects were trained to respond to blue and yellow as labels for addition and subtraction.
When studying animal cognition, the training element is key, says聽Rosa Rugani, a postdoctoral researcher at the University of Pennsylvania.听Being聽capable聽of using symbols and聽naturally聽using them in their own wild environments are two different things.
鈥淎nimals, to survive in their natural environment, they don鈥檛 need to reach this level of abstraction,鈥 Dr. Rugani says. They don鈥檛 need to have 鈥+鈥 or 鈥-鈥. They just need to be able to gauge food resources, keep track of their group, navigate, and assess the threat of a pack of predators. So it may be more of an indication of an animal鈥檚 ability to learn labels, rather than a mind for math.
To separate out those two things, Rugani has studied number cognition in newborn domestic chicks. Her results have suggested they may have a concept of a聽聽and can tackle some聽.
So what sets humans apart?
So are we humans predispositioned to have our numerical capabilities, or is it a learned or trained behavior?
Looking to language for clues, researchers found that some hunter-gatherer languages around the world have words for numerals only up to the number five 鈥 or even fewer, says Professor N煤帽ez of UC San Diego. These societies聽just haven鈥檛 had .听
This suggests that humans鈥 mathematical abilities have been culturally developed as societies found a use for them, N煤帽ez says. 鈥淚t鈥檚 not that, because we鈥檙e humans, we [automatically] get math,鈥 he says.
But there must be something that sets humans apart, says聽Jessica Cantlon, a professor at Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh who co-authored the聽 with Brannon while at Duke University. Perhaps the combined use of symbols and precision work together to enable people to process complex mathematical concepts.
Her research has compared humans with other primates, and found that the two are matched in what they can do with approximate numbers up until the humans turn 3. Then, human children hit a threshold and are better able to understand the precision of numbers.
鈥淲hat lets us access that kind of thinking?鈥 Professor Cantlon says. 鈥淚 don鈥檛 know. But that鈥檚 what everybody would like to know.鈥
N煤帽ez thinks it鈥檚 a matter of having a few different general capacities (which he calls 鈥渂iologically evolved preconditions鈥) and the cultural scaffolding to put them all together.
Regardless of how it all came together, Dyer says, 鈥淚 think it would be a reasonably widely accepted perspective that mathematics is a primary thing which underpins the development of technology, and is very important to the success of modern humans.鈥